Response to Allington and Woodside-Jiron
Patricia G. Mathes
University of Texas – Houston Health Science Center
Joseph K. Torgesen
Florida State University
(in press, Educational Researcher)
Abstract
The program of research on reading sponsored by the
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD)
is one important source of information for practitioners
choosing best practices for early literacy instruction. On the
basis of evidence from both normal and disabled readers, this
research indicates that multiple factors are critical to early
literacy success and that phonological knowledge and skill is
one such element. Recently, the utility of this body of
research has been called into question by Allington and
Woodside-Jiron in an article titled, “The Politics of Literacy
Teaching: How ‘Research’ Shaped Educational Policy." This
response to that article is written to examine policy making
activities that are misrepresented, to correct mistakes made
by Allington and Woodside-Jiron about the range and utility of
NICHD research, to correct possible misperceptions of the
NICHD-supported research effort that might arise in the minds
of those who read the Allington and Woodside-Jiron article,
and to present new evidence that clarifies the utility of much
of this research.
The Response
According to the recent report from the National Research
Council titled “Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young
Children”(Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998), excellent early
general education reading instruction is critical to the
prevention of the majority of reading difficulties. This
report clearly states that unless effective instructional
practices are identified and broadly applied in
practice, the instructional inequities that commonly divide
our children into the elite high performers and rejected low
performers is certain to continue. The program of research on
reading sponsored by the National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development (NICHD) is clearly one important source
of information that can assist practitioners in choosing
effective practices.
This research and other research funded by other
sources should be used to guide practice in much the same way
as research supported by other branches of the National
Institutes of Health are used to guide fields such as medicine
and pharmacology. For example, recent NICHD research findings
indicating the importance of infant sleeping positions in
reducing the incidence of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS)
has been enthusiastically embraced by the medical community
and dissemination efforts are well underway. Unfortunately,
the findings from NICHD-supported research that are relevant
to the prevention of reading failure have not been similarly
embraced by the educational community. Instead, we find
serious efforts to discredit and misrepresent these findings
(Coles, 1998, Goodman, 1998; Taylor, 1998).
The utility of this research for guiding decisions about
instruction was recently called into question by Allington and
Woodside-Jiron in the recent article appearing in the
Educational Researcher titled, “The Politics of Literacy
Teaching: How ‘Research’ Shaped Educational Policy.” Our
purpose in writing this response is to examine policy making
activities that are misrepresented, correct mistakes made by
Allington and Woodside-Jiron about the range and utility of
NICHD research, and present new evidence that clarifies the
utility of much of this research. For purposes of clarity, and
in order to avoid too much repetition of names, we will refer
to Allington and Woodside-Jiron throughout the paper as AWJ.
Likewise, we refer to NICHD funded reading research as NICHD-RR.
In their article, AWJ state that they “are particularly
interested in discerning how advocates advance a specific
policy agenda and the role ‘research’ plays in the advocacy”
(p. 4).
However, they never actually follow through with their
stated purpose. Instead, they focus on one paper by Bonita
Grossen titled, “Thirty Years of Research: What We Now Know
About How Children Learn to Read” that appeared on the Center
for the Future of Teaching’s Webpage. They treat this paper as
if it were the primary catalyst of reform within several
states (i.e., California and Texas). They then purport to
examine the support available for the conclusions about
reading instruction made by Grossen. However, this examination
takes the form of discounting the utility of the entire 30
years of research on reading, reading development, and reading
instruction that has been financially supported by the
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD).
One of our purposes in writing this response to AWJ is to
examine the weight the Grossen paper has had in shaping
educational policy and to put into perspective the
interpretations and misinterpretations of NICHD-supported
research made by Grossen (1997). It is important to understand
that the NICHD reading research program and the Grossen
article are not one in the same. Without this awareness, it is
very easy to accept AWJ’s criticisms of the Grossen article as
applicable to the entire 30-year NICHD research base. In our
response, we deal separately with issues related to NICHD-sponsored
research and Grossen’s interpretation of this research.
A core statement made by AWJ is that the NICHD
recommendation that early reading instruction include
explicit, systematic teaching of the alphabetic principle,
including reading practice using decodable text, is not based
on research relevant to general education. At the root of
their argument is a simple discounting of the findings of
NICHD reading research as unimportant. They accomplish their
agenda by:
1. Misrepresenting the scope of NICHD reading research as
focusing only on children with reading disabilities.
2.Disowning general education’s responsibility to children
who experience reading difficulties.
3. Confusing the issue of credibility and utility of sound
research with political advocacy.
4. Discounting and misrepresenting the importance of many
outcomes of NICHD intervention research.
5. Over emphasizing Grossen’s (1997) interpretation of the
large and complex NICHD research base as simply incorrect and
as having a large influence on state reading policies.
We will examine each of these arguments in turn, finding
their conclusions largely inaccurate. We hope this additional
information and discussion will be helpful to all who are
sincerely interested in advancing our understanding of factors
and instructional practices that may be critical in helping
all children become good readers.
We have attempted to write this response in a tone that
will foster rational discussion rather than heated and/or
personal argument. We believe that both AWJ and ourselves are
sincerely interested in improving the instructional
experiences and reading growth of children for whom reading is
difficult. We are also equally committed to the methods of
science as one means of achieving our aims in this area, and
that commitment demands that we be as fair as possible in
examining all the evidence relevant to the issues at hand. At
present, we have decidedly different points of view on some of
these issues; it is our hope that respectful exchange of views
will help us all to move to a more accurate and useful
synthesis of the knowledge than is presently available.
It should also be noted that this response to AWJ is based
on the most recent 1999 variant of the manuscript that
appeared in Educational Researcher. We must admit that
we find it amusing that AWJ discuss the difficulty they had in
writing their critique of the Grossen paper because variants
of that paper were found on the CTFL WebPages and in published
form in Effective School Practices. We found this complaint
amusing because we too experienced difficulty in writing our
critique because we too found 3 versions of the AWJ paper that
were similar but not identical. Versions of the AWJ article
include “Adequacy of a Program of Research and of a ‘Research
Synthesis’ in Shaping Educational Policy,” which was made
available on the National Research Center on English Learning
and Achievement’s WebPage (Allington & Woodside-Jiron, 1997).
A second, shorter, but very similar article was published in
chapter form (Allington & Woodside-Jiron, 1998), and of
course, the most recent reiteration published in
Educational Researcher (Allington & Woodside-Jiron, 1999).
Background and Context
Before examining AWJ’s arguments, we first frame the
context for this discussion. Unfortunately, we live in an era
where the former “great debate” is now characterized as the
“reading wars” (Goodman, 1998). Fueling the flames of this
“war” is converging empirical evidence on one side (see the
recent report from National Research Council edited by Snow et
al., 1998) and heated philosophical rhetoric on the other (see
Coles, 1998; Goodman, 1998; Taylor, 1998). The unfortunate
casualties of this war are the large number of children who
experience significant problems becoming competent readers.
In the midst of this debate, there have been repeated calls
to reform reading instruction. The result is that typical
reading instruction that occurs in today’s primary grade
classrooms is qualitatively different than it was even a
decade ago (Allington, 1994; Vaughn, Moody, & Schumm, 1998).
Today we have an educational community in which many
elementary grade and special education teachers have traded in
basal reading series for "literature anthologies" that still
look much like basal reading series, where ability group
instruction has been replaced with whole class
undifferentiated instruction, where specific skills are only
"taught as needed," and where many teachers believe that
"reading should be caught not taught" and that teaching
phonics is harmful to young children (Mathes, 1998; Vaughn et
al., 1998). Unfortunately, little has changed in terms of
academic outcomes over the last 20 years (Snow et al., 1998)
and the gaps between children of middle class families and
children of poor families have remained fairly constant for
the past 16 years (National Center of Educational Statistics,
1996). Presently, over 40% of 4th and 8th graders fail to read
at levels considered essential to performing grade level work
(National Center of Education Statistics, 1996), and very poor
readers consistently make little to no reading gains from year
to year (Klingner, Vaughn, Schumm, Hughes, & Elbaum, 1998;
Schumm, Vaughn, Elbaum, 1996; Vaughn et al., 1998; Zigmond, et
al, 1995). Thus, it appears that we are educating only some,
not all, our children to become good readers (Snow et al.,
1998).
What Do NICHD Supported Scientists who Study
Reading Actually Advocate?
Converging evidence from research at 41 sites nationwide
supported by NICHD as well as research supported by other
sources such as the U.S. Office of Education has lead reading
researchers supported by NICHD to repeatedly conclude that
teaching the alphabetic principle is one critical
component for preventing or remediating reading failure for
many children.
What NICHD supported reading researchers do not
advocate is code-only instruction, a focus on skill
worksheets, skills taught in a disjointed manner, or skills
taught only in isolation. Likewise, NICHD reading researchers
do not suggest that beginning readers should be
deprived of authentic literature and read only decodable text.
What they, as a group, do advocate for is balance (see
Fletcher and Lyon, 1998 for a more detailed summary)
Lyon summarizes the NICHD research base in
the following manner.
There is evidence suggesting that a constellation of
components configured in an instructional reading program
that if used properly for a sufficient period of time will
contribute to the reading acquisition of many children. At
present, these components include instruction in phonemic
awareness, the alphabetic principle, the development of
speed and automaticity in word and text reading, and the
development of reading comprehension strategies within a
literature rich environment. The relative importance of
using decodable text and predictable text formats for
readers who differ on each of these components is not yet
well understood and NICHD research is now underway to
explicate this issue. It is conceivable that a combination
of both types of text formats will be necessary to foster
adequate development at different states of reading.
What is inconceivable is that the issue of decodable text
stirs such emotion given the tremendous amount of evidence
from research on transfer and generalization of skills, in
general. The data have shown and continue to show that
generalization of basic skills to other contexts and the
development of speed and automaticity in applying skills
across contexts and formats is critically dependent upon
opportunities that provide ample representation of the
concept being acquired. This is particularly true of those
who have difficulty acquiring a skill. (Lyon, August, 1997,
personal communication).
Much Ado About Nothing
We are perplexed why AWJ have chosen to focus attention on
the Grossen, 1997 article. From the AWJ portrayal of Grossen’s
30-year paper, one would think that this one paper was largely
responsible for shaping the entire early literacy reform
efforts within California and Texas. The truth is much
different. In our examination of policy efforts, we have
determined that in both California and Texas the Grossen
article had little or no impact on shaping educational policy.
The reality is that in California, the Grossen 30 years
paper postdated passage of key California early reading
reform legislation. In Texas, when the Grossen paper first
appeared, leaders within the Texas Educational Agency (TEA)
recognized that Grossen’s interpretation of NICHD research did
not adequately portray the large and complex NICHD research
base and the document was not used as a guiding document for
the state’s reading initiative. We know these things because
we examined the timelines for policy activities within each
state, interviewed policy leaders who were involved in reading
policy making within each state, and reviewed state produced
documents; actions we are surprised that AWJ did not
undertake. Thus, it appears that AWJ miscalculated the effect
of the Grossen paper by assuming that wide distribution of the
paper via the Internet equated to large impact on policy
without checking the validity of their assumption. As a
result, AWJ’s argument about how “research” was misused in
California and Texas is fundamentally flawed. Furthermore, the
Grossen paper, to our knowledge, has not been credited by any
group, other than AWJ, as being a consensus document or a true
synthesis of research. We would rather reserve this
designation for scholarly reports that merit it such as the
recent NRC report (Snow et al, 1998).
Reading Reform in Texas
In Texas, the roots of advocacy for the inclusion of
systematic, explicit instruction in the alphabetic principle
as one component of a balanced reading program began well over
a decade ago, when the Nation At-Risk report was released
(National Commission on Excellence in Education, 1983). At
that time, the first author of this paper was actually a
teacher of reading in that state, and remembers seeing copies
of the report passed around and discussed in a variety of
venues, including state conferences, faculty meetings, and
even IEP meetings. By 1988, Texas implemented its dyslexia
reading legislation for all children who were falling severely
behind in reading. Services provided under this legislation
are not part of special education or Title 1 funds, but rather
are funded by the state. The cornerstone of these services is
code-emphasis instruction in reading for children who were
falling behind their classmates in reading growth. The state’s
attention has only more recently focused on general education
classrooms, following recognition that general education
reading instruction was failing too many children.While not
legislated as in California, the current reform effort began
in Texas in 1996 when Governor Bush allocated funds to the
Texas Education Agency (TEA) for the state’s current Texas
Reading Initiative. As a first order of business, TEA
developed the reading initiative’s guiding document, Beginning
Reading Instruction: Components and features of a
Research-Based Reading Program (Texas Education Agency, 1996).
What this report recommends is balanced reading instruction.
Specifically, it states that children should have
opportunities to: (a) understand and manipulate the building
blocks of spoken language, (b) learn about and manipulate the
building blocks of written language, (c) learn the
relationship between the sounds of spoken language and the
letters of written language, in addition to many other
elements such as opportunities to write and relate their
wiring to spelling and reading and opportunities to read and
comprehends a wide assortment of books and other texts. The
research and reports cited for this document reveal that (a)
Grossen’s work was not utilized, and that (b) the work of many
researchers were included – some with funding from NICHD, most
with funding from other sources.
The next task of the Texas Reading Initiative was to
develop new standards for teaching reading that were
consistent with the Beginning Reading Instruction document by
replacing the former Texas Essential Elements with the Texas
Essential Knowledge and Skills (TEKS). TEKS focuses on
providing balanced reading instruction and were developed
without referring to the Grossen paper. Since that time, the
reading initiative has been focused on staff development
efforts.
Reading reform in California
California’s movement toward advocating for a more balanced
reading curriculum is more recent than in Texas. In the
1980’s, holistic, literature-based practices were prescribed
in the state’s Language Arts Framework, and explicit
code-instruction banned. This framework was reaffirmed in 1987
leading publishers to produce the now common “literature
based” reading series. By 1992, when NAEP scores were
released, California’s reading scores were low. By 1994, NAEP
scores were even lower. Over 10 years, the state had had
plummeted from near the top to near the bottom in national
comparisons (National Center for Educational Statistics,
1996). Furthermore, the drop in reading scores could not be
accounted for by the state's changing demographics. Arguably,
it was the 1994 NAEP scores more than anything else that lead
to California’s reading reform movement.
In 1995, Delaine Easton, a democrat, became California’s
State Superintendent of Public Schools. Under Easton’s
leadership, the state put together a reading task force. In
September of 1995, 2 years before the Grossen paper, the task
force released a guiding document for the state’s reading
reform. From the California Reading Task Force report, “it was
determined that a balanced and comprehensive approach to
reading must have: (1) a strong literature, language, and
comprehension program that includes a balance of oral and
written language; (2) an organized, explicit skills program
that includes phonemic awareness (sounds in words), phonics
and decoding skills to address the needs of the emergent
reader; (3) ongoing diagnosis that informs teaching and
assessment that ensures accountability; and (4) a powerful
early intervention program that provides tutoring for children
at risk for reading failure” (California Department of
Education, 1995, p.2). In that same year, the legislature
became involved, passing Assembly Bill 1504 (ABC Bill)
mandating explicit teaching of phonics, spelling, and math.
The legislature then passed Assembly Bill 170 (Baldwin Bill)
reiterating the call to teach explicit, systematic phonics as
one part of the early reading curriculum. Again, this
legislation occurred before the Grossen paper had been
written.
In May 1996, again prior to the Grossen paper, the
California Department of Education released Teaching Reading:
A Balanced, Comprehensive Approach to Teaching Reading in
Prekindergartern though Grade Three (California Department of
Education, 1996). This program advisory endorsed vigorous
early reading instruction including phonemic awareness and
explicit systematic phonics as well as instruction and support
in vocabulary, reading comprehension, and regular broad,
thoughtful reading. Once released, it was quickly adopted by
the State Board of Education and the Commission of Teacher
Credentialing.
In July of 1996, Assembly Bill 3482 provided money for
staff development for teachers and administrators in effective
early reading, which included, among other things, staff
development on teaching systematic, explicit instruction in
the alphabetic principle. In all, the California legislature
passed eleven pieces of legislation during 1995-1996, all of
which were aimed at the same content (the incorporation of
phonemic awareness and alphabetic knowledge instruction into
the curriculum). All were passed unanimously by both
republicans and democrats and all occurred prior to or
simultaneously with the release of the Grossen paper.
Influence of NICHD-RR
Beyond the 30 years paper, NICHD supported Reading Research
(NICHD-RR) is portrayed by AWJ as a “policy lever” (p. 5) that
is used by those pushing a “code-emphasis” agenda. Reid Lyon
and NICHD supported researchers are presented as political
advocates of this agenda because they provided expert
testimony to various policy-making bodies in California,
Texas, and elsewhere. In our view, AWJ greatly overemphasize
the role of NICHD supported researchers in stimulating the
advocacy for reading reform in California and Texas. While it
is accurate to say NICHD supported research influenced
decision-makers, as it should, it is inaccurate to present
this research program as the primary influence. For instance,
in California, NICHD staff and researchers were brought into
the state rather late in the reform process. Likewise,
numerous individuals provided testimony to that’s state’s
legislative body. In fact, Allington himself was one of the
first reading experts brought in by the state to help shape
policy. In reality, the majority of individuals who provided
expert assistance to California’s policy making bodies were in
no way affiliated with NICHD and included Marilyn Adams,
Richard Allington, Isabel Beck, Robert Calfee, David Chard,
Jane Fell Greene, Connie Juel, Adria Klein, Gay Su Pinnell,
Nancy Roser, Linda Seigel, Dale Willows, John Shefelbine, and
Robert Slavin among others. Given that these individuals have
been funded by sources such as the National Science
Foundation, The Office of Educational Research and Improvement
within the U.S. Office of Education, and private foundation
monies, it is simply wrong to characterize NICHD funded
research and researchers as having the greatest influence in
the state. Additionally, in both California and Texas,
movement away from a holistic orientation started many years
ago at the grass roots level, typically by upset parents
appalled by the lack of reading ability they were observing in
their own children.
In sum, for AWJ to imply that NICHD-supported researchers
have been pushing their agenda on states is a serious
misrepresentation. The researchers they attack have conducted
scholarly research in which various approaches have been
contrasted. The findings are reported according to the rules
of scholarship, which are designed to guard against bias. The
fact that NICHD-supported researchers, as a group, support the
inclusion of explicit-systematic code instruction has come
about because this is the approach supported by the
preponderance of data.
One role of any good researcher is to disseminate his or
her findings, to put them in the public domain, so that that
they can be examined and challenged. However, good science
dictates that challenge come in the form of further research
-- experimentation, not rhetoric. Likewise, when there is a
convergence of findings, such as has recently occurred with
regard to the importance of explicitly teaching the alphabetic
principle, with multiple researchers, in multiple contexts,
and with a variety of different kinds of subject and research
designs, then responsible scientific practices dictate that
the findings be actively disseminated. This is not advocacy,
this is science.
Arguments Undermining the Usefulness of
NICHD-Supported Research in Reading
The Scope of NICHD Reading Research
AWJ misrepresent the scope of the NICHD program of reading
research as narrowly focused only on children with reading
disabilities and as having an a priori focus on phonological
core deficits as a primary etiology of reading difficulties.
The NICHD research program has been much more extensive than
either the Grossen or AWJ articles indicate. NICHD has funded
research on a variety of learning processes at all levels
including molecular, cellular, behavioral, neurological and
educational, using a variety of animal and human models. For
more than 30 years, the program has initiated and supported
studies that examine early brain development, relationships of
language and reading in non-impaired children and adults,
social and biological factors in literacy, and other areas
that impact reading, but don’t involve disability. The more
recent intervention and prevention studies, initiated in 1993
for children with reading disabilities and selectively cited
by both Grossen and AWJ, are built on this larger body of
research first initiated in 1965.
About 50% of NICHD supported reading research has been
devoted to studies on how language, reading, and
reading-related processes develop in proficient readers; the
other half has examined factors that impede normal acquisition
of these skills. Thus, even if the NICHD-supported reading
disability intervention studies had never been conducted, and
certainly if the Grossen paper had never been written,
findings from NICHD-supported research on reading would still
be an important source of information with implications for
instruction.
Importantly, the two types of NICHD supported reading
research (i.e. normal development and reading disabilities)
converged independently to show the critical role of
alphabetic, or phonetic, reading skills both as a support for
normal emergent reading and as an explanation for reading
failure in many children (for an excellent discussion of this
convergence, see Share & Stanovich, 1995). There is also a
very powerful convergence of evidence about the nature of word
reading processes in skilled reading (Adams, 1990; Raynor &
Pollatsek, 1989) with studies of the word reading difficulties
of poor readers (Morrison, 1987; Torgesen, in press). This
convergence has helped to provide a focus for many of the
NICHD-supported intervention studies on methods for preventing
and remediating word-level reading problems.
It is of further importance to understand that the
convergence of findings in NICHD-supported research on reading
(as well as its convergence with much research not supported
by NICHD) was not guided by any systematic, centralized plan
among administrators and scientists at NICHD. Typically,
research ideas are submitted for support to NICHD where
scientists from around the country review them for scientific
adequacy. NICHD administrators do not play a major role in
deciding which studies receive funding and which do not. This
decision is made based primarily on scientific merit.
Recently, under the direction of Dr. Reid Lyon, NICHD has
escalated its investment in research on reading and reading
disabilities. However, the major role that Dr. Lyon has played
in guiding the research is to request proposals in the general
areas of intervention research as well as research that
involves co-ordinated, multi-disciplinary efforts to
understand the nature, development, and etiology of reading
disabilities. It is important to understand that the questions
addressed, measures selected, and design of the studies
themselves is all controlled by individual investigators who
are not employees of NICHD.
Responsibility for All Children
AWJ argue that NICHD-RR should not be used to guide reform
of early literacy curriculum, which, as they point out, is the
curriculum of “virtually all children” (p. 2). They ignore the
fact that half of NICHD-RR has been conducted on typically
developing readers, and assert that the NICHD-RR has not been
conducted with general education’s children in general
education classrooms. This argument seems to suggest that
research conducted on general education’s lowest performing
20% has no bearing on how reading should be taught; implying
that children who experience reading difficulties are not the
responsibility of general education.
So who are these children for whom AWJ deny responsibility?
They are children sitting in general education classrooms
being taught by general education teachers who typically have
not been taught how to teach reading to children who do not
learn to read easily (Moats, 1995). Typically, after being
allowed to fail, these children receive labels such as
learning disabled, mildly retarded, emotionally disturbed, and
attention-deficit-disorder. Some of these children are blamed
for their failure to learn because they are limited in their
English proficiency, come from poor families, or are simply
assumed to be "slow-learners." Most importantly, they are
often children who can learn to read at roughly normal levels
if provided the right kind of instruction (Foorman, Francis,
Fletcher, Schatschneider, & Mehta, 1998). Interestingly, these
are the children about whom Allington, in his other writings,
appears to be most concerned (see Allington 1994, 1997).
The majority of students who have reading problems are
really what can be termed ‘curriculum casualties.’ In large
part, these students are poor and of minority status. In
total, children experiencing reading problems represent as
much as 40% of the total school population (Snow et al.,
1998). For these children, we argue that the NICHD-RR provides
their teachers with guidance about how to promote their early
and continued reading success (Ehri, 1998). To ignore the
needs of such a sizable portion of children serves only to
promote an underclass trapped in a cycle of illiteracy,
prevented from expressing their true potential. For these
children, the NICHD-RR has much to say to teachers and program
developers about how to provide equitable reading education.
Equitable reading instruction demands the incorporation of
research-informed critical components such as explicit,
systematic instruction in the alphabetic principle for those
children who require it. Equitable reading instruction may
well mean providing differentiated instruction for different
groups of children within the same classroom, or even
providing one-on-one tutoring (Torgesen, 1998). Equitable
reading instruction absolutely requires that teachers be
knowledgeable in principles and techniques for how to
effectively teach critical content. Thus, equitable reading
instruction necessitates reforming general education reading
instruction to include those elements that are critical to
insure adequate reading growth in children who do not learn to
read easily (i.e., systematic, explicit instruction applying
the alphabetic principle: Snow et al., 1998).
Measures Used in NICHD Reading Studies
One cornerstone of AWJ’s arguments is a disregard for the
findings of recent intervention research based on a
philosophical belief that the measures used in much of this
research aren’t important to “real reading” and therefore do
not count. We acknowledge that the measures used in the NICHD-supported
intervention research conducted to date are quite different
than the “authentic literacy assessment” that is currently
advocated by many reading professionals (Paris et al., 1992).
“Authentic assessment” attempts to judge children’s ability to
apply broad literacy skills to authentic tasks like gathering
information for a report, use of literacy as a medium for
social interactions, or ability to read a selection and then
write a response to it. It also attempts to assess children’s
enjoyment, ownership, and involvement in literacy activities
both at school and at home. We would certainly not argue
against this type of assessment as part of an overall
evaluation of reading outcomes for children. After all, if a
child can read, but does not apply important literacy skills
effectively to every day tasks or reads simply for the joy of
reading, then some important goals of literacy instruction
have not been attained. However, there must be recognition
that for many children, the problem is not one of failure in
applying reading skill, but rather the lack of reading skill
itself. In contrast to what are often referred to as
“authentic measures” of literacy, the recent intervention
studies supported by NICHD typically use more objective,
quantitative measures of specific reading skills. These
measures are chosen because they have demonstrated reliability
and validity for predicting academic performance. In contrast,
“authentic measures” typically have no known reliability or
validity and are often subjective.
A focus of NICHD-RR has clearly been to evaluate carefully
the growth of word-level reading skills. The rationale for
this focus is the vast preponderance of evidence indicating
the serious impact that failure to acquire critical word
reading abilities has on overall reading growth for children
who end up as poor readers at the end of elementary school
(Francis et al., 1996; Juel, 1988; Torgesen & Burgess, 1998).
These data suggest that, whatever else a particular
intervention accomplishes for at-risk children, if it does not
have a significant impact on their ability to read words
fluently and accurately in text, then it has not addressed a
primary problem they experience in becoming good readers. In
other words, attention to reading individual words matters if
we want our children to successfully apply broad literacy
skills to authentic tasks like gathering information for a
report, use of literacy as a medium for social interactions,
or ability to read a selection and then write a response to
it.
AWJ poke fun at one of the commonly used measures in NICHD-supported
research by referring to it as “psuedo-word pronunciation.”
The clear implication of their comments is that these measures
do not assess real reading skills. On these types of tasks,
children are asked to read a non-word such as “surt.” The
purpose is to assess the child’s ability to decode using the
alphabetic principle. Real words and connected text are not
used so that the child must rely on alphabetic knowledge
rather than memory or context cues. There is now voluminous
evidence that difficulties applying phonetic decoding skills
on non-word reading tasks such as the ones used in NICHD-RR is
one of the most important defining features of the reading
performance of children who experience serious problems
learning to read (Bruck, 1990; Rack, Snowling, & Olson, 1992;
Siegel, 1989; Siegel & Faux, 1989; Stanovich & Siegel, 1994).
AWJ also misrepresent results about reading comprehension.
They state that "the NICHD studies that have implemented
direct instruction have found it easier to enhance phonemic
awareness and non-word pronunciation performances than to
influence word reading, reading rate or fluency, or
comprehension achievement" (p.6). What they don't say is that
word reading, reading rate, and comprehension, while harder to
influence, are influenced. Likewise, the studies they choose
to discuss are all kindergarten studies--studies in which
children were not yet independently reading text. They
discount studies where comprehension gains have been
impressive. For example, Foorman et al. (1998) found an effect
size of .76 on reading comprehension for gains with children
in Title 1 schools receiving direct, balanced code instruction
in the general education classrooms.
Another recent study (Torgesen et al., 1999) provided
preventive interventions to children identified in
kindergarten as the 12% most at-risk for reading failure. This
study showed that the most phonemically explicit of three
instructional interventions was the only one to produce
significant growth in word level skills beyond that attained
by children in a no treatment control group. When contrasted
against the other two intervention groups, the most explicit
condition had an effect size for phonemic decoding of .95 and
for real word identification of .50. Although individual
contrasts for passage comprehension between the most explicit
and less explicit conditions were not statistically reliable,
the effect size for comprehension was .43, which is
educationally meaningful. In this study, the condition that
provided the most intensive, explicit, and systematic
instruction in word-level reading skills produced stronger
growth in reading comprehension than the other two conditions,
in spite of the fact that these latter conditions contained
more opportunities to read and discuss text for meaning.
Although the authors concluded that, had there been more time
for instruction, children in the group receiving the most
explicit word-level instruction would have profitted from more
opportunities to engage in meaningful experiences with print,
the study is nevertheless instructive concerning the
importance of powerful word-level instruction for children
at-risk for reading failure.
It is important to notice that AWJ present no alternative
measures that could provide objective evidence for the
efficacy of specific intervention methods. If AWJ know of
measures that have been demonstrated to be more reliable and
valid than those used in NICHD-RR, it would be very
interesting to compare them directly to the measures that are
currently being used. Allington did communicate to Grossen
that he would need to see enhanced achievement on reading
comprehension, composition, reading response, or voluntary
reading and writing before he would be inclined to recommend a
specific method (Allington, November 1997, personal
communication). We agree that all those outcomes are desirable
for an overall literacy program, but we do not feel that this
minimizes the importance and meaningfulness of more
fundamental measures of early reading progress. Is it
meaningful that children who are nonreaders at the beginning
of a study learn how to read words they have never encountered
and that they read connected text accurately? Is being able to
read the individual words printed on a page of text likely to
increase or decrease comprehension of that text? Would a child
who can independently read words never before encountered be
more or less likely to read voluntarily? Would a child who
understands how the sounds of language map onto printed words
using the alphabetic principle be more or less likely to write
voluntarily?
Grossen’s Interpretation of NICHD
supported reading research
In general, we feel Grossen’s paper has been greatly
misrepresented. We concur with AWJ and Lyon and Fletcher
(1998) in stating that the specific recommendations go beyond
the actual NICHD research base. However, the recommendations
are not completely out of line with the research base. The
issue is really one of specificity of the recommendations.
NICHD-supported research does not specifically tell
practitioners how to teach early reading; instead it provides
indications about the critical elements of effective
instruction. In other words, the research reports describe
systematic, explicit instruction as a critical component of
instruction for children experiencing difficulties. Grossen
went beyond the database in defining the specific procedures
that should be used in implementing “systematic and explicit”
instruction. NICHD researchers have used a variety of
treatment packages and, as a group, they have not recommended
exactly how to provide explicit, systematic instruction in the
alphabetic principle. For example, NICHD-supported research
has not determined an exact order in which grapheme-phoneme
relationships should be taught, but does indicate that being
systematic about the order of presentation results in better
outcomes. Similarly, the optimal balance and timing for use of
decodable text in early reading instruction has not been
specifically determined, but use of decodable text has been
incorporated as one feature of interventions that have
assisted children in making the greatest achievement gains. It
is important to keep in mind that Grossen’s article was not
intended as an all inclusive, comprehensive synthesis of the
research. Rather, it was designed to communicate to
practitioners basic tenets of good instruction as gleaned from
the NICHD reading research program. To put this paper in
context, it must be remembered that the majority of teachers
and school administrators have little knowledge about how to
effectively teach alphabetic knowledge and skills (Moats,
1995; Lyons, 1995). Likewise, terms like “systematic, explicit
phonics” are not widely understood.
In our own research, which has involved working with and
observing in hundreds of classrooms, we have been struck
repeatedly by the numbers of dedicated teachers who do not
know even the basics of teaching the alphabetic principle such
as the importance of pronouncing sounds the way they are
typically pronounced in real words, or the need to show
children how to sound out words. Our observations are
consistent with recent estimates that only about 1 in 10
teachers has the necessary expertise to teach reading to
children experiencing reading difficulties (Lyon, 1995). Thus,
we believe that, while not particularly scholarly, the Grossen
article represents an often overlooked necessary link in the
chain of dissemination. It provides a practical look at the
types of activities used by NICHD researchers that have
resulted in greater reading gains for children experiencing
reading difficulties. The 7 implications of NICHD research the
Grossen article identifies are:
- Begin teaching phonemic awareness directly at an early
age.
- Teach each sound-spelling correspondence explicitly.
- Teach frequent, highly regular sound-spelling
relationships systematically.
- Show children exactly how to sound out words.
- Use connected, decodable text for children to practice
sound spelling relationships they learn.
- Use interesting stories to develop language
comprehension.
- Balance [decodable text and authentic stories], don’t
mix.
In this discussion, we comment only on those
implications that AWJ discussed (i.e. implications 2,3,5 and
7).
Implication 2: Teach each sound-spelling
correspondence explicitly
The essential question here is whether or not it is
beneficial for children to be explicitly and systematically
taught the connections between sounds and letters in words
while they are learning to read. When the question is stated
at this level, it is apparent that AWJ agree with most NICHD-supported
researchers about the benefits of instruction in the
alphabetic principle. Where AWJ most directly challenge the
support for Grossen’s statements about explicit instruction in
sound-spelling correspondences concerns specific methods that
can be used to provide this instruction. Whereas Grossen
recommends methods that introduce sound-spelling
correspondences systematically outside the context of
meaningful reading or writing activities, AWJ support less
systematic explication of these relationships within the
context of activities like writing where they are taught in
the service of a goal involving meaningful communication. AWJ
simply do not believe that the results of the cited NICHD
studies are meaningful.
However, the NICHD-supported studies cited by Grossen are
clear that interventions that provide more systematic explicit
instruction in sound-spelling correspondences produce better
reading growth than those that do not. Perhaps the most
convincing of these studies to date was recently reported by
Foorman et al. (1998). In this study, the investigators
contrasted instructional methods involving explicit
instruction in alphabetic reading skills with methods that
contained less explicit and systematic instruction in the
code. Within methods that contained explicit instruction in
sound-letter correspondences, they contrasted two
interventions that varied in whether instruction was provided
at the single phoneme level or the onset-rime level. The
research was conducted in Title 1 schools in intact first- and
second-grade general education classrooms. The children who
received the most complete and explicit instruction in
sound-letter correspondences at the single phoneme level
outperformed children in the other instructional conditions on
multiple measures including phonetic decoding, word
identification, and passage comprehension. In AWJ’s portrayal
of this study, the reader is left believing that no meaningful
effects were detected because the groups did not perform
differently from one another on one of the two measures of
reading comprehension. As the authors explained, and as the
data suggested, the comprehension measure on which the groups
did not differ (i.e., Formal Reading Inventory -
Comprehension) was simply too difficult for children in first
and second grade; this measure was not sensitive to growth for
any of the children in any of the groups. The explicit code
group did demonstrate greater comprehension on a measure at
the appropriate level of difficulty (ES = .76).
We are also concerned that the second author’s work (Torgesen,
Wagner, & Rashotte, 1997a) is quoted out of context as
indicating that direct instruction in phonics does not
generalize to orthographic reading and reading comprehension.
In the study that this quote pertains to, all the intervention
programs involved some degree of direct instruction in phonics
and they also all produced growth in sight word and passage
comprehension skills. The method that involved the most
explicit instruction produced the greatest gains in these
areas, although the differences for passage comprehension
across instructional methods were not statistically
significant. Perhaps the most important finding of this study
involved the overall success of children identified in
kindergarten as the most at-risk for reading failure, and thus
grade retention. The children who had been randomly assigned
to the most systematic and explicit instructional condition
had only a 9% retention rate between kindergarten and second
grade, while the children in the other two experimental
conditions had a 20% to 25% retention rate and the control
children showed a 40% retention rate. The point of the
Torgesen et al. (1997a) study is that with early intervention
that contains explicit and systematic instruction in
alphabetic reading skills, phonologically-based reading
problems can, in most instances, be prevented. The major
evidence for this statement comes from the finding that the
word-level reading skills of these children (which is a
critical area of difficulty for these children) were solidly
in the average range for both fluency and accuracy at the end
of the study.
Scanlon and Vellutino’s (1997) research is also
misrepresented as demonstrating that “NICHD-supported
researchers indicate letter-sound instruction in early school
environments was not the critical instructional difference in
fostering early reading achievement in the at-risk children
studied” (p. 7). In contrast, what Scanlon and Vellutino
(1997) concluded is that kindergarten teachers typically are
oriented toward more holistic practices and that “children who
enter kindergarten at-risk for reading difficulties, but who
are good readers by the end of first-grade...come from
kindergarten classrooms in which a greater portion of time is
spent in analyzing the structural…aspects of the spoken and
written word” (p. 209). What we find most disconcerting about
AWJ’s presentation of this study is they choose to ignore the
discussion pertaining to the observed inadequacy of
exclusively holistic, meaning-based activities for
kindergarten children.
We were particularly concerned with AWJ’s treatment of the
study by Lovett, Borden, DeLuca, Lacarenza, Benson, and
Brackstone (1994). What this study indicated was that teaching
sound-symbol relationships explicitly was superior to implicit
approaches with older, severely deficient readers. The point
was not that they were still behind, but that they made
greater progress with one technique over another. Given that
these students were already so far behind, finding the tools
to help them rapidly achieve more skills is very important,
even though they did not achieve 4-years growth during the
treatment period, which was actually quite short in this
study. Researchers are trained to present their results in a
conservative manner, not overstating their implications. The
conservative nature of good researchers should not be used as
tool to discredit their findings. Achieving positive results
with such challenging students should not be written off as
insignificant.
A second problem that AWJ have with implication 2 is that
Grossen cites 14 references, half of which are commentaries or
reviews, rather than original studies. Given that Grossen's
purpose was to communicate to practitioners what NICHD
researchers recommend based on their research, we see no
problem with citing the summaries and commentaries of those
researchers, in addition to specific studies. Likewise, if
practitioners are interested in gathering the original
studies, the original citations are located in the reviews.
Since practitioners are less likely to possess the skills
necessary to comprehend the original research, reviews may
well be more appropriate.
Implication 3: Teach frequent, highly
regular sound-spelling relationships systematically
The problem that AWJ have with this recommendation appears
to be that the published studies, which are restricted by page
limitation, do not provide detail about which sound-symbols
were taught and if the sound-symbol correspondence taught to
children represented the most frequent sound represented by
the letter. It is true that the NICHD researchers have not
explained their treatments at this level of detail in their
published manuscripts. However, this does not mean that the
implication is incorrect. What it does mean is that the
researchers assume that those who are interested in this type
of specificity will themselves examine the materials used
within their research. The fact is that those treatments that
have achieved the best results have taught first the sounds
that appear in words more frequently and have typically
presented the most regular sound spelling relationships as
described in Grossen’s article. To determine this, one must
examine the actual curricula that achieved the best results.
Also, Grossen’s list of 48 sounds is intended to be
informational, not specific to any one curriculum. Our
experience in working with teachers is that the information
presented in the table is not common knowledge. Grossen is
very clear that these letter-sound correspondences presented
are not from any one program or curriculum, and are only
presented to provide the readers with adequate background
knowledge. Thus, we do not find this table problematic because
it does not have any bearing on any of the research, but
rather is helpful for understanding the research.
Implication 5: Use connected, decodable
text for children to practice sound spelling relationships
they learn
AWJ state that, “None of the NICHD-supported studies
isolated or manipulated the decodable text variable” (p. 9).
They are correct, but this is not the most important point.
The most effective treatments in all of the NICHD-supported
intervention studies involved the use of decodable text at
some point in the instruction. It is from this fact that
Grossen rightly indicates that NICHD research supports the
idea that decodable text is an important component of
successful reading programs.
The reason the researchers have not isolated decodable text
as a distinct component of instruction is that it has been
considered a logical component of any intervention that seeks
to strengthen the kind of word reading skills that do not
involve guessing from context. The words in text cannot be
directly identified (either through phonetic analysis or
orthographic recognition) until the elements comprising the
text have been taught. Thus, decodable text must be linked to
a total program of teaching letter-sounds, sounding-out
skills, and sight words before it becomes decodable. Second,
in light of what is known about transfer and generalization,
it makes no sense to teach letter-sound correspondences,
sounding-out skills, and sight words strictly in isolation. In
fact, to deliberately deprive children of reading text
designed to facilitate the integration of these skills would
be, in our opinion, unethical. Quite frankly, we are not sure
why AWJ take issue with having children read decodable text to
promote transfer, generalization, and meaningful integration
of alphabetic skills when they, themselves, acknowledge that
it is important for children to acquire these skills. To teach
skills in isolation, without the text, is reminiscent of the
isolated skill and drill phonics of the past-- practices
Allington advocates against (Allington, 1994, 1997). Likewise,
to ask children to read text that they cannot decode using the
alphabetic elements and skills they have been taught, is to
communicate to them that the alphabetic knowledge and skill
they have spent effort learning is not really relevant to
reading, and that they must rely heavily on guessing the
identity of words from context.
Implication 7: Balance, don’t mix
Here, AWJ are responding to Grossen’s (1997) statement
that, “A common misconception regarding the balance that is
called for by the research is that the teacher should teach
sound-spelling relationships in the context of real stories”
(p. 13). Grossen is making a very similar point about
instruction and practice here as she did in implication 5. The
essential question is how to reinforce the use of phonetic and
visually based word-reading skills in young children. Should
they be given opportunities to practice reading in texts that
allow them to identify most of the words on the basis of
phonetic and visual information in the text, or should they
practice reading texts that require them to guess a high
proportion of the novel words from context? AWJ are correct in
stating that NICHD-RR has not directly examined the proper
balance between decodable text and “real stories” in the
reading selections given to young children. However, Grossen's
recommendation that young children who are beginning to learn
to read should be given both kinds of texts for different
purposes (decodable text to practice independent word
identification skills, “real stories” to experience in
supported-reading or teacher-reading contexts) is consistent
not only with the recent synthesis of research provided by the
National Research Council (Snow et al., 1998) but with a large
body of both NICHD-RR and research supported by other sources
(Gough and Walsh, 1991; Raynor & Pollatsek, 1989; Share &
Stanovich, 1995).
We do want to take direct issue with one of the comments
made in the AWJ paper relevant to the issue of “balance.” In
attempting to show that NICHD-RR research is inconsistent with
Grossen’s recommendations, they cite the testimony of Reid
Lyon (1997), who is the director of the branch that supports
much of the research, as stating, “NICHD researchers have
found that classroom instruction that explicitly addresses the
connections between letters and sounds within a
literature-rich classroom environment can make a difference
between reading failure and reading success.”(p.4). What Dr.
Lyon was attempting to communicate here is that many children
will learn to read much better if they are given systematic
and explicit instruction and practice using phonetic and
visually-based word reading strategies in a context that also
provides rich opportunities to interact with meaningful text.
He was arguing that neither of these important components
should be left out. He did not exclude the use of decodable
text as a device for practicing word-reading skills, and yet
that is the clear implication made by AWJ in their discussion
of this citation.
Long Term Impact
Since AWJ wrote their critique, new important research has
been released that speaks to the long-term impact of at least
some of the intervention research that we feel bears on the
utility and generalizability of the NICHD-funded research.
Thus we include a brief description here. Presently other
longitudinal research examining long-term impact is underway.
In a recent study of remedial instruction with children in
late elementary school, Torgesen and his colleagues (Torgesen,
Alexander, Wagner, Rashotte,Voeller, & Conway, 1999) found
that explicit instruction in phonemic decoding skills, coupled
with opportunities to apply those skills effectively in
reading text, produced dramatic and lasting gains in the
reading ability of children who began the study with severe
reading disabilities. For example, the children began the
study with an average standard score (Mean = 100, S.D. = 15)
for phonemic decoding ability of 69. After 67.5 hours of
one-to-one tutorial instruction, their score on this test was
93, and a two-year follow-up (during which they had received
no additional intervention) showed their average score to be
91. For comprehension of text, the children began with a
standard score of 81, with an immediate posttest score of 90,
and a two-year follow-up score of 96. Since this group had a
measured verbal IQ of 93, their final performance on reading
comprehension was slightly higher than one would predict on
the basis of general verbal ability.
Conclusions
In truth, we are not clear why the NICHD-RR should be so
vigorously attacked when what is actually espoused by this
research and these researchers is so reasonable, data-based,
and non-exclusionary. The community of scholars whose work in
reading has been supported by NICHD has not made any specific,
one-size-fits all recommendations. All that has been posited
is that, based on all available evidence from both normal and
disabled readers, it appears that multiple factors are
critical to early literacy success, of which phonological
knowledge and skill is one such element. The research suggests
that, in order for reading instruction to be maximally
effective for all children, it should contain explicit
instruction in phonetically and visually-based word-reading
strategies--teaching strategies that have been explicitly
rejected in the holistic approaches to reading instruction
that have been widely utilized in recent years. Allington
himself appears to actually support many of the same positions
that NICHD supported researchers advocate (see Allington,
1994).
It seems to us an unnecessary exercise in futility to
attack the utility of sound research and present a bewildering
and misleading picture to practitioners. We are very worried
that such activities confuse practitioners who are left
believing that they can still pick and choose reading
approaches as if choosing food at a cafeteria. Likewise, the
constant arguing between "experts" can only leave
practitioners distrustful. We propose that what "experts"
should be engaged in is acknowledging commonalties and
continuing to examine, scientifically, the impact of
particular practices for which questions remain.
We would challenge AWJ, and others who are critical of the
instructional recommendations derived from NICHD-RR, to
conduct equally well-controlled investigations of
instructional methods. Although there is clearly room for
discussion about what constitutes evidence of instructional
effectiveness, we would assert that, at a minimum, the
measures used should have demonstrated reliability and
validity. In order to make valid systematic comparisons among
methods that can be replicated by others, at least a core of
objective measures, that provide quantitative data to be used
in systematic analyses, should be used. Although case studies
and descriptive research have much to contribute in terms of
explaining the context and conditions of observed effects, it
must also be recognized that these research tools cannot
distinguish the efficacy of various methods or approaches and
should not be given the same weight as research that
experimentally manipulates instructional variables under
relatively well-controlled conditions.
As for the Grossen (1997) article, we reject the position
that this one paper has had wide spread impact on policy in
California and Texas. Instead, we take it for what it is, a
practitioner-oriented article designed to communicate some
basic principles of instruction that are consistent with what
NICHD-RR suggests are important for early literacy instruction
designed to prevent reading failure. The recommendations and
examples included were not intended to be definitive. We
believe that Grossen was well justified to present examples
because, in our own experiences working with teachers and
administrators, it has become clear to us that these
practitioners are sometimes confused by the language
researchers use and find specific examples very helpful.
We agree with Allington (1994) when he states that
“children are more likely to learn what they are taught than
what they are not” (p. 1). We agree with Allington that “it is
simply not necessary that some children fail to learn to read
well” (p.2).
Without a strong instructional component children are left
to their own devices to discover the strategies and processes
that skillful readers and writers use. Many children attempt
to puzzle through the activities but never discover the
thinking patterns that skillful readers employ. We now label
these children and schedule them for special instructional
programs. It is time, instead, to teach them what they need to
know. (Allington, 1994, p. 10.) This statement is completely
consistent with evidence gathered by NICHD supported
researchers over the past 30 years. However, before we can
teach children what they need to know, we must know what it is
they need to know. Thus, we must look to data, not dogma, for
guidance. If we are ever to achieve equity in reading
instruction for all children, we must, as a field, use
findings of high quality research as they become available to
help us design and implement the best quality literacy
instruction to “teach them what they need to know.”
References
Adams, M.J. (l990). Beginning to read:
Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Adams, M.J. (1998). The three-cueing system.
In F. Lehr and J. Osborn (Eds.). Literacy for All: Issues in
Teaching and Learning (pp. 73-99). New York: Guilford Press.
Allington, R.L. (November, 1997). Personal
communication. [Electronic mail from R. Allington to B.
Grossen, forwarded by B. Grossen to the P. Mathes]
Allington, R.L. (1997). Reducing the risk:
Integrated language arts in restructured elementary schools.
(Report series 1.9) Albany: National Center of English
Learning and Achievement and Learning.
http://www.albany.edu/cela/reducerisk/index.html
Allington, R.L. (1994). Schools we have:
Schools we need. (Report series 1.2) Albany: National Center
of English Learning and Achievement and Learning.
http://www.albany.edu/cela/reducerisk/index.html
Allington, R.L., & Woodside-Jiron, H. (1997).
Thirty years of research on reading: Adequacy and use of a
“research summary” in shaping educational policy. (Report
series 1.15) Albany: National Research Center of English
Learning and Achievement.
http://www.albany.edu/cela/allington/index.html
Allington, R.L., & Woodside-Jiron, H. (1998).
Thirty years of research on reading: When is a research
summary not a research summary. In K. Goodman (Ed). In defense
of good teaching, York, ME: Stenhouse.
Brown, F.S., & Felton, R.C. (1990). Effects of
instruction on beginning reading skills in children at-risk
for reading disability. Reading and Writing: An
Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 223-241. Bruck, M. (1990).
Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of
dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439-454.
California Reading Task Force. (1995). Every
child a reader: The report of the California reading task
force. Sacramento: California Department of Education.
California Department of Education. (1996).
Teaching reading: A balanced comprehensive approach to
teaching reading in prekindergarten through grade three.
Sacramento: Author.
Clay, M. (1991). Reading recovery: A guidebook
for teachers in training. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Coles, G. (1998). Reading lessons: The debate
over literacy. New York: Wang & Hill. Ehri, L.C. (1998).
Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read
words in English. In J. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.). Word
recognition in beginning reading. (pp. 3-40). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.
Elley, R. (1992). How in the world do students
read? Hamburg: The Hague International Association for the
Evaluation of Educational Achievement.
Elley, R. (1992). How in the world do students
read? Hamburg: The Hague International Association for the
Evaluation of Educational Achievement.
Fletcher, J.M., & Lyon, G.R. (1998). Reading:
A research-based approach. In W. M. Evers (Ed.), What’s Gone
Wrong in America’s Classrooms (pp. 49-90). Stanford, CA:
Hoover Institute Press.
Francis, D.J., Shaywitz, S.E., Stuebing, K.K.,
Shaywitz, B.A., & Fletcher, J.M. (1996). Developmental lag
versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal,
individual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 88, 3-17.
Foorman, B.R., Francis, D.J., Beeler, T.,
Winikates, D., & Fletcher, J.M. (1997a). Early interventions
for children with reading problems: Study designs and
preliminary findings. Learning Disabilities: A
Multi-Disciplinary Journal, 8, 63-71.
Foorman, B.R., Francis, D.J., Fletcher, J.M.,
Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998). The role of
instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in
at-risk children, Journal of Educational Psychology, 90,
37-55.
Foorman, B.R., Francis, D.J., Winikates, D.,
Mehta, P., Schatschneider, C., & Fletcher, J.M. (1997b). Early
Interventions for children with reading disabilities.
Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 255-276.
Goodman, K. (1998). In defense of good
teaching, York, ME: Stenhouse.
Gough, P., & Walsh , S. (1991). Chinese,
Phoenicians, and the orthographic cipher of English. In S
Brady & D. Shankweiler, (1991). Phonological processes in
literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Grossen, B. (1997). 30 years of research: What
we now know about how children learn to read: A synthesis of
research on reading from the National Institute of Child
Health and Development. Santa Cruz, CA: The Center for the
Future of Teaching and Learning.
http://www.cftl.org
Haynes, M.C., & Jenkins, J.R. (1986). Reading
instruction in special education resource rooms. American
Educational Research Journal, 23, 161-190.
Honig, B. (1997). Reading the right way: What
research and best practices say about eliminating failure
among beginning teachers. The School Administrator, 54, 15-20.
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A
longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth
grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437-447.
Klingner, J.K., Vaughn, S., Schumm, J.S.,
Hughes, M., & Elbaum, B. (1998). Academic outcomes for
students with and without learning disabilities in inclusive
classrooms. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 13,
Lovett, M., Borden, L., DeLuca, T., Lacerenza,
L., Benson, N., & Brackstone, D. (1994). Treating core
deficits of developmental dyslexia: Evidence of transfer of
learning after phonologically- and strategy-based reading
training programs. Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 805-822.
Lyon, G.R. (August, 1997). Personal
communication. [electronic mail from R. Lyon to R. Allington,
carbon copied by R. Lyon to B. Grossen, forwarded by B.
Grossen to the P. Mathes]
Lyon, G.R. (1995). Research initiatives in
learning disabilities: Contributions from scientists supported
by the National Institute of Child Health and Development.
Journal of Child Neurology 10, S120-S126 (Supplement 1). Lyon,
G.R. (July, 1997). Statement of G. Reid Lyon, Ph.D., before
the Committee on Education and the Workforce, U.S. House of
Representatives. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health.
Moats, L.C. (1995). The missing foundation in
teacher education. American Educator, 19, 9-19.
Morrison, F.J. (l987). The nature of reading
disability: Toward an integrative framework. In S. Ceci (Ed.),
Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects
of learning disabilities (pp. 33-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mathes, P.G. (1998). [Preventing Early Reading
Failure by Enhancing Classroom Technologies - Project PERFECT,
U.S. Department of Education Grant #H180G60004]. Unpublished
raw data
Mathes, P.G., and Torgesen, J.K. (in press).
All children can learn to read: Critical care for the
prevention of reading failure. Peabody Journal of Education.
National Center of Educational Statistics.
(1996). National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP)
1994 reading report card for the nation and status (Report #
96045). Washington, D.C.: Author.
National Commission on Excellence in
Education. (1983). A nation at risk: the imperative for
educational reform. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
Office.
Paris, S. G., Clafee, R. C., Filby, N.,
Hiebert, E. H., Pearson, P. D., Valencia S. W., & Wolf, K. P.
(1992). A framework for authentic literacy assessment. The
Reading Teacher, 46, 88-98.
Rack, J.P. Snowling, M.J., & Olson, R.K.
(l992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia:
A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29-53.
Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (l989). The
psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice
Hall.
Scanlon, D.M., & Vellutino, F.R. (1997). A
comparison of the instructional backgrounds and cognitive
profiles of poor, average and good readers who were initially
identified as at risk for reading failure. Scientific Studies
of Reading, 1, 191-216.
Siegel, L.S. (1989). IQ is irrelevant to the
definition of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 22, 469-479.
Siegel, L.S., & Faux, D. (1989). Acquisition
of certain grapheme-phoneme correspondences in normally
achieving and disabled readers. Reading and Writing: An
Interdisciplinary Journal, 1, 37-52.
Share, D. C., & Stanovich, K.E. (1995).
Cognitive processes in early reading development: A model of
acquisition and individual differences. Issues in Education:
Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1-57.
Snow, C.E., Burns, M.S. & Griffin, P. (1998).
Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Stanovich, K.E., & Siegel, L.S. (1994). The
phenotypic performance profile of reading-disabled children: A
regression-based test of the phonological-core
variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology,
86, 24-53.
Taylor, D. (1998). Beginning to read and the
spin doctors of science: The political campaign to change
America’s mind about how children learn to read. Urbana, IL:
National Council of Teachers of English.
Texas Education Agency (1996). Components and
features of a research-based reading program. Austin: Author.
Torgesen, J.K. (1998). Instructional
Inteventions for Children with Reading Disabilities. In S.
Shapiro, D. Accardo, and C. Capute (Eds). Specific Reading
Disability: A View of the System. Parkton, MD: York Press.
Torgesen, J.K. (in press). Reading
disabilities. In R. Gallimore, A. Bernheimer, G. MacMillan, D.
Speece, & S. Vaughn (Eds.). Developmental Perspectives on
Children with High Incidence Disabilities: Papers in honor of
Barbara
K. Keogh. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. Torgesen, J.K., Alexander, A., Wagner, R.K.,
Rashotte, C.A., Voeller, K., & Conway, T. (1999). Immediate
and long-term growth in response to intensive remedial
instruction in reading. Manuscript in preparation, Florida
State University, Tallahassee, Fl.
Torgesen, J.K. & Burgess, S.R. (1998).
Consistency of reading-related phonological processes
throughout early childhood: Evidence from longitudinal-correlational
and instructional studies. In J. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.).
Word
Recognition in Beginning Reading. (pp.
161-188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Torgesen, J.K.,
Wagner, R.K., & Rashotte, C.A. (1997a). Prevention and
remediation of severe reading disabilities: Keeping the end in
mind. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 217-234.
Torgesen, J.K., Wagner, R.K., Rashotte, C.A.,
Alexander, A.W. & Conway, T. (1997b). Preventive and remedial
interventions for children with severe disabilities. Learning
Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8, 51-61.
Torgesen, J.K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C.A.,
Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T. , & Garvin, C. (1999).
Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological
processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to
instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 1-15.
Vaughn, S., Moody, S.W., & Schumm, J.S.
(1998). Broken promises: Reading instruction in the resource
room. Exceptional Children, 64, 211-225.
Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M., Sipay, E.,
Small, S., Pratt, A., Chen, R., & Denckla, M. (1996).
Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily
remediated poor readers: Early intervention as a vehicle for
distinguishing between cognitive and experimental deficits as
basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 88, 601-638.
Ysseldyke, J.E., Thurlow, M.A., O’Sullivan,
P., & Christenson, S.L. (1989). Teaching structures and tasks
in reading instruction for students with mild handicaps.
Learning Disabilities Research, 4(2), 78-86.
Zigmond, N., Jenkins, J., Fuchs, L.S., Deno,
S., Fuchs, D., Baker, J.N., Jenkins, L., & Couthino, M.
(1995). Special education in restructured schools: Findings
from three multi-year studies. Phi Delta Kappan, 76(7),
531-454.